Skip to main content

Interaction between cognitive leisure activity and long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid intake on global cognitive decline in a Japanese longitudinal cohort study: National Institute for Longevity Sciences-Longitudinal Study of Aging

Abstract

Background

There is a growing interest in the significance of adopting a variety of lifestyle habits for maintaining cognitive function among older adults. A lifestyle that is easy to modify, simple, and less burdensome for older people is ideal. We investigated the longitudinal association between global cognitive decline and cognitive leisure activities (CLAs) combined with long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs) intake.

Methods

The National Institute for Longevity Sciences-Longitudinal Study of Aging (NILS-LSA) enrolled community-dwelling middle-aged and older men and women who were randomly selected from Obu-City and Higashiura Town, Aichi, Japan. Baseline data (2006–2008), including CLAs and dietary intake, were obtained from 517 participants (aged 60–84 years) with normal cognition. Global cognitive decline, defined as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score ≤ 27, was assessed at baseline and four years later. Interaction between CLAs and LCPUFAs on cognitive decline was investigated using a multiple logistic analysis with adjustment for confounders. CLA engagement and LCPUFA intake were divided into high and low groups according to the frequency at which each participant engaged in the activity and the median intake level according to sex, respectively.

Results

A significant interaction was detected for the combination of CLA engagement and LCPUFA intake. Logistic regression coefficients revealed significant interactions when participants engaged in more than five CLA varieties. One of the CLAs, art appreciation, produced a significant main effect against cognitive decline and a significant interaction in combination with LCPUFA intake. The major LCPUFAs—docosahexaenoic acid and arachidonic acid—also exhibited a significant interaction. The combination of high LCPUFA intake and high art appreciation frequency yielded a lower adjusted odds ratio for cognitive decline than the combination of low LCPUFA and low art appreciation [0.25 (95 % confidence intervals, 0.11–0.56)].

Conclusions

Preserving cognitive function might be associated with a combination of varied and high-frequency engagement in CLAs combined with high LCPUFA intake.

Peer Review reports

Background

Dementia is a serious public health problem worldwide. The latest World Health Organization guidelines recommend adopting healthy lifestyle behaviors, such as physical activity, smoking cessation, and a balanced diet, for preserving cognition [1]. Recent clinical trials investigating interventions for cognitive preservation have focused on the value of addressing lifestyle practices with multidomain interventions [2,3,4]. However, the choice of specific lifestyle factors to combine remains unclear due to the inconsistency in the results across studies and the availability of interventions.

Older people have more time to engage in leisure activities compared with younger individuals. Physical activity is often recommended as a preventive measure for preventing cognitive decline [1]. However, cognitive leisure activities (CLAs) with lower physical burdens may be more suitable for older adults. A systematic review defined CLAs as activities for enjoyment or well-being that cause intellectual stimulation and suggested the improvement of cognitive function through CLA interventions [5]. The CLAs assessed in the study included arts, writing, and board games. A meta-analysis of observational studies suggested that CLAs lowered the dementia risk not only among people with good cognitive health, but also among those with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [6, 7].

Diet is another modifiable lifestyle factor. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs), such as docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), and arachidonic acid (ARA), are important nutrients present in marine and animal food products that contribute to maintaining health. In addition, LCPUFAs are major components of the brain. Despite the importance of brain LCPUFAs, their levels have been reported to decrease with age [8,9,10]. In animal studies, LCPUFAs were shown to be involved in the regulation and maintenance of brain function, such as neural membrane fluidity, synaptic plasticity, and neurogenesis [11,12,13,14,15,16,17]. Moreover, some intervention studies have indicated that supplemental LCPUFAs improve cognitive function in older adults [18,19,20].

The Finger Study is a recent and well-known clinical trial that evaluated cognitive function maintenance using a multidomain intervention, including fish consumption (rich in LCPUFAs) and cognitive training as interventions [2]. A combination of activation and nutrients could be crucial in maintaining cognitive function. As LCPUFAs levels in the brain decrease with age and play an important role in cognition, we hypothesized that LCPUFA supplementation combined with engagement in activities that stimulate cognitive function is more effective in maintaining cognitive function in older adults than practicing either alone. In other words, the combination of two beneficial lifestyle factors may produce an additive or synergistic effect on maintaining cognitive function in older people.

However, no epidemiological studies have addressed the impact of the combination of CLAs with LCPUFAs on cognitive decline. In this longitudinal study, we focused on the association between CLA combined with LCPUFA intake and cognitive decline in community-dwelling Japanese older adults.

Methods

Study design

The data analyzed in the present longitudinal study were obtained from the National Institute for Longevity Sciences-Longitudinal Study of Aging (NILS-LSA), a Japanese population-based, prospective cohort study. Participants in the first wave (1997–2000) of the NILS-LSA included ~ 2,300 men and women aged 40–79 years [21]. Participants were followed up every 2 years until the seventh wave of the examination, which was completed in 2012, and a postal survey is ongoing. Participants included randomly selected, age- and sex-stratified individuals from Obu-City and Higashiura Town in Aichi Prefecture. When participants could not attend the follow-up investigations, the same number of age- and sex-matched random samples were recruited, excluding individuals aged > 79 years.

Study population

Baseline data were collected from the fifth wave survey (2006–2008), which included 2,419 men and women aged 40 years or older. The follow-up period was 4 years, representing the period from the fifth to the seventh wave (2010–2012). We excluded the following individuals: age ≤ 59 years at baseline because the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is assessed only in older participants (age ≥ 60 years); those who failed to participate in MMSE assessment at the fifth and seventh wave survey; those with MMSE scores ≤ 27 at baseline; those with a self-reported history of previously diagnosed dementia at baseline; and those with missing data at baseline for adjusted variables in the statistical analysis. By excluding those who failed to meet the inclusion criteria or participate in the follow-up MMSE assessment, 517 participants were included in this analysis (Fig. 1). The Committee of Ethics of Human Research of the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology approved this study (approval number:1322), and all methods were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participants included in this longitudinal study. MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; NILS-LSA, National Institute for Longevity Sciences-Longitudinal Study of Aging; N, number

Cognitive assessment

Cognitive function was assessed using the Japanese version of the MMSE [22, 23]. The MMSE, which is a brief measure of global cognition and a screening test for dementia, is the most commonly administered test in clinical and research settings. Trained psychologists and researchers administered the test at each wave. The MMSE score ranges from 0 to 30, with higher scores indicating better cognitive function. The participants in the NILS-LSA (age ≥ 60 years) were relatively highly educated [24], and it has been determined that the standard MMSE cut-off score of 23/24 would not provide an adequate classification of highly educated older individuals [24,25,26]; a higher cut-off score of 26/27 or 27/28 has been recommended for highly educated individuals [25]. Therefore, we defined cognitive decline as an MMSE score of ≤ 27.

Cognitive leisure activities

The items of the questionnaire for leisure activities in the NILS-LSA were developed by referring to various administrative surveys and a study [27] that surveyed the leisure activities of older adults. CLAs vary according to country and culture. A Japanese group, the Japanese longitudinal cohort study of Septuagenarians, Octogenarians, and Nonagenarians Investigation with Centenarians, was comprehensively surveyed and various Japanese leisure activities were categorized [28]. Reportedly, a categorized group, named “hobby activities” was associated with decreased MCI risk [28]. The “hobby activities” part of the questionnaire used in the above Japanese cohort includes six leisure activities, which correspond to the questionnaire used in NILS-LSA. The six CLAs were reading newspapers, reading books, writing (e.g., diary writing as a hobby), creative activities (e.g., calligraphy, painting, photography, and Japanese dressmaking), art appreciation (e.g., theater visits, watching movies, and music appreciation), and gardening (e.g., home gardening and Japanese bonsai gardening). A research psychometrist assessed the frequency with which each participant engaged in each CLA using a questionnaire survey with the following responses: “never,” “once or several times a year,” “once or several times a month,” “once a week,” “several times a week,” or “every day.” Participants were asked to provide information about their engagement in these activities during the year before study participation.

Nutritional assessment

Nutritional intake per day, including mean energy intake and fatty acid intake, was assessed using a 3-day dietary record after participation. The dietary record was completed over three continuous days (2 weekdays and 1 day of the weekend) [29]. Most participants completed the records at home and returned them within one month. Either the food was weighed separately on a kitchen scale before being cooked or portion sizes were estimated. Participants took photographs of their meals before and after eating. The average 3-day food and nutrient intake was calculated according to the Standard Tables of Food Composition in Japan, 2010 [30]. Alcohol intake in the previous year was assessed using a food-frequency questionnaire., We focused on three fatty acids, DHA, EPA, and ARA, as representative LCPUFAs because of their relatively high intake.

Other measurements

The questionnaires were collected on the examination day. Both weight and height were measured on the examination day, to calculate body mass index (BMI). Trained interviewers used a questionnaire for assessing physical activity regarding the intensity and frequency of activity over the preceding year [31]. The mean amount of total physical activity per day was calculated (metabolic equivalents [MET], MET × min/day). The depressive tendency was assessed using the Japanese version of the CES-D questionnaire [32, 33]. Participants completed the CES-D questionnaire at home to assess their level of depression in the previous week and brought it with them on the examination day. A self-completed questionnaire, which was provided approximately 2 weeks before the examination day, was used to collect information on the participant’s history of hypertension, hyperlipidemia, ischemic heart disease, stroke, and diabetes (yes/no); education (≤ 9, 10–12, or ≥ 13 years of school); employment status (no occupation or household labor, irregular employment, and regular employment); marital status (yes/no); and smoking status (yes/no).

Statistical analyses

All analyses were conducted from 2019 to 2020 using the Statistical Analysis System version 9.3 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). The confounding variables were sex; age; MMSE score; BMI; education level; marital status; smoking status; alcohol consumption; energy intake; physical activity; employment status; depressive tendency (assessed using the CES-D score); and history of hypertension, hyperlipidemia, ischemic heart disease, stroke, and diabetes at the baseline. Differences in characteristics, frequency of CLA, and fatty acid intake among participants with and without cognitive decline were assessed using the chi-squared test (categorical variables) or Student’s t-test (continuous variables). Fisher’s exact test was used as an alternative to the chi-squared test if the expected counts obtained were < 5. For the analysis of interactions, we divided CLAs into two groups according to the frequency of participation; “at least once or more per month” (high); “once or several times per year or less” (low) [34]. We established high and low LCPUFA groups based on the median LCPUFA intake according to sex. First, the association between cognitive decline and each of the two exposures, CLAs and LCPUFA intake, was estimated using the main effect model. In addition, the total number of CLAs that the individual engaged with high frequency (high) was calculated. Apart from detection of the interactions between each CLA and the intake of each fatty acid, we also investigated the interactions between LCPUFA intake and the total number of CLAs in which the individuals engaged with high frequency. Furthermore, we performed a multiple logistic regression analysis and estimated the odds ratios (ORs) and 95 % confidence intervals (CIs) for the three combinations compared with lower frequencies and intakes. In model I, we adjusted for age and sex. Model II was further adjusted for the above-mentioned confounding variables. Two-sided P values < 0.05 were regarded as statistically significant.

Results

Baseline characteristics

The mean age was 68.6 (SD, 6.2) years, and 141 (27.3 %) participants showed a cognitive decline after 4 years. Without adjusting for confounders, participants who developed cognitive decline were significantly older, had lesser regular employment, and had lower MMSE scores at baseline (Table 1). More than half of the total participants frequently engaged in CLAs, such as reading the newspaper, reading books, writing, and gardening. The percentage of participation in creative activities and art appreciation was lower than in the remaining four CLAs. Engagement in art appreciation was significantly different between those with and without cognitive decline. Participants with cognitive decline also had a significantly lower intake of saturated fatty acids and MUFAs than those without cognitive decline. The median values of LCPUFA intake that segregated the two groups (high and low) were 1,412 mg/day (men) and 1,071 mg/day (women).

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the participants according to the presence or absence of 4-year period of cognitive decline

Interaction between cognitive leisure activities and intake of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids according to the total number of cognitive leisure activities

Table 2 shows the interaction between the level (high and low) of LCPUFA intake and total variety of CLAs with a high frequency of participation (at least once or more per month). All participants participated with a high frequency in at least one or more out of the six CLAs. No significant interaction was found between the number of CLAs for those who participated in ≥ 1 to ≥ 4 CLAs and LCPUFA intake. The logistic regression coefficients for the interactions between the total variety of CLAs and the level of LCPUFA intake were significant at − 1.003 (SE, 0.507) when performing ≥ 5 activities (P = 0.048).

Table 2 Interaction between CLA and LCPUFA intake according to the total number of CLAs

Main effect of each cognitive leisure activity and intake of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids on cognitive decline

One of the CLAs, art appreciation, showed a statistically significant main effect after adjusting for other potential confounders; however, none of the other CLAs exhibited a significant main effect (Table 3). Another exposure, LCPUFA intake, also showed a significant main effect after adjusting for other potential confounders [0.62 (95 % CI, 0.40–0.98)]. DHA, EPA, and ARA did not produce significant main effects [DHA: 0.75 (95 % CI, 0.48–1.18), EPA: 0.73 (95 % CI, 0.47–1.14), ARA: 0.69 (95 % CI, 0.44–1.10)]. In addition, a significant main effect on cognitive decline was detected when the total number of CLAs engaged in by the participant was six [0.43 (95 % CI, 0.19–0.97)].

Table 3 Main effect of each CLA on cognitive decline

Interaction between the intake of each long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid and each cognitive leisure activity on cognitive decline

Interaction of art appreciation with each LCPUFA, except EPA, was also observed (Table 4). Reading the newspaper was excluded from the table because it was in a quasi-complete separation state in the analysis. Among the five activities, art appreciation showed a significant interaction with LCPUFA, DHA, and ARA intake. Furthermore, the interaction between art appreciation and LCPUFA was significant at various delimitations of frequency, not only “once or several times a month or more” but also “once a week” (p = 0.0131) and “several times a week” (p = 0.0341).

Table 4 Interaction between each CLA and intake of each fatty acid

Odds ratios for cognitive decline according to the exposure combination

The exposure for the combination was art appreciation and each LCPUFA intake. The ORs of each combination were calculated using a category group as a reference, i.e., a low combination in the case of a low frequency of art appreciation and low fatty acid intake. In the crude model, the group with the combination of a high frequency of art appreciation and high intake of LCPUFA, DHA, EPA, and ARA had a significantly lower OR for cognitive decline. These significant associations were retained even in model I after adjusting for sex and age, and in model II after adjusting for all covariates (Table 5).

Table 5 Odds ratios (ORs) for cognitive decline according to the exposure combination

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate the potential impact of the interaction of varied and high-frequency engagement in CLAs with high LCPUFA intake on the preservation of cognitive function in community-dwelling older individuals. One of the CLAs, art appreciation, showed a significant interaction in combination with LCPUFA intake. In the main effect model, both art appreciation and LCPUFA intake showed significant associations with cognitive decline, respectively. The combination of a high frequency of art appreciation and high LCPUFA intake yielded a lower OR for cognitive decline compared with a low combination of the two parameters. DHA, EPA, and ARA exhibited the same tendency.

Several clinical research articles have shown that CLAs are more effective for the preservation of cognitive function than physical or social activities [28, 35]. According to animal studies, the learning inability and brain β-amyloid deposition were suppressed in cases involving complex play equipment in addition to exercise compared with the exercise-only group [36]. Cognitive function gradually declines in older people, who have fewer opportunities to use their brains [37]. Continued engagement in CLAs in later life may stabilize or activate cognitive function [38]. Previous studies have shown that frequent participation in various activities that provide cognitive stimulation was associated with a reduced risk for Alzheimer’s disease [39]. In line with this, the present study found that participation in six CLAs had a significant main effect on cognitive decline. As such, CLAs alone may prevent cognitive decline, provided that patients frequently participate in various CLAs.

Art appreciation creates a dialog with others and invites the exchange of impressions; thus; art appreciation could be considered a social activity as well as an intellectual activity. Since the relationship between social connections and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease pathology has been reported, the social activity component of art appreciation may also be reflected in our results [40, 41]. Art appreciation could also positively influence emotions and mood. Overall, it was observed that those who engaged in five or more CLAs and those who frequently engaged in art appreciation tended to have healthier lifestyles than those with less frequent participation in these activities. One recent study reported longitudinal associations between receptive arts engagement and mortality [42]. Another study suggested that positive emotions help control cardiovascular systems linked with dementia risk [43]. An interventional study showed that exercise combined with music compared with exercise alone preserved cognitive function in older people [44]. Therefore, it is inferred that the positive emotions derived from art appreciation contribute to cognitive function.

The brain’s emotional areas, such as the amygdala and the entire orbital insular cortex, are activated during art appreciation [45, 46]. According to a study, the same areas of the brain were reportedly active during both visual and auditory means of art appreciation [47]. Another study highlighted the importance of hippocampal−orbital insular cortex interactions [48]. Brain regions that are connected to emotions or memory function, such as the orbitofrontal cortex or hippocampus, respectively, showed lower LCPUFA levels with aging [8,9,10]. LCPUFAs are well-known cannabinoid precursors. The function of the brain’s endogenous cannabinoid system diminishes with normal aging [49,50,51,52]. According to a recent report, the administration of low-dose cannabinoids to young mice impairs the ability to perform learning and memory tasks; however, the same dose can improve the ability to perform learning and memory tasks in aged mice [53]. Therefore, the reduction of LCPUFA levels that accompanies aging may influence the cognitive function of the part of the brain that is responsible for emotions and memory. Art appreciation may activate emotional parts of the brain that are linked to the memory parts.

A significant interaction between art appreciation and EPA was not observed in the present study. EPA certainly is less abundant in the brain than are DHA and ARA; however, a meta-analysis demonstrated that EPA may be more effective than DHA in the treatment of mood disorders, such as depression [54]. Each LCPUFA might provide an original contribution to the preservation of cognitive function.

The combination of CLA engagement and LCPUFA intake is more effective in preventing cognitive decline than either intervention alone. These two interventions may support the cognitive reserve in different ways, mimicking the functions of software and hardware [55]. LCPUFA intake supplements brain components that decline with age, contributing to the maintenance of structural stability (hardware), whereas CLAs strengthen neural circuit functions (software); thus, the functions of the hardware can be considered fully utilized because of the existence of software. As such, CLAs that can continue to functionally activate various areas of the brain would be a useful leisure activity for the brain.

Strengths and limitations

The study participants were randomly selected among community dwellers based on resident registrations. Accurate nutritional assessments were based on 3-day dietary intake and were recorded using a scale and photographs. However, the limitations of this study should be considered. First, our findings may not be applicable to other countries or cultures because the present results were obtained using the Japanese cohort population. Japanese people consume plenty of fish and shellfish, which are the primary sources of EPA and DHA [56, 57]. The EPA and DHA intake levels might contribute more to the LCPUFA intake level because ARA intake is similar among different countries [57]. Japanese people may have a higher LCPUFA intake than other populations. Moreover, CLAs vary widely between countries due to cultural differences. However, art appreciation may be a universal activity if the esthetic sense is a commonality among different cultures.

Second, it is unclear whether the delimitations and category of the CLAs used in this study are typical or reasonable. The delimitation and frequency of activities differ from one report to another. In the present study, the interaction between LCPUFA and art appreciation was significant in various delimitations. However, since the validity of the activity delimitations has not been established, we cannot refer to a specific activity frequency. Another serious limitation is that the frequency of activities was uniform across all CLAs in this analysis. For example, reading books once a month or more is not considered as a high frequency. Dividing the frequency of activities by the median number of people is one method for addressing this challenge; however, some items, such as newspapers, cannot be divided evenly. Furthermore, there is substantial variation between reports regarding separations for different types of activities. Therefore, we conducted our analysis according to the delimitations described in a report that analyzed items of intellectual activities similar to those assessed in our study by uniformly dividing the frequency of activities into two parts [34]. As for the classification of CLAs, we developed our questionnaire with reference to a study that surveyed leisure activities in older adults [27] and various administrative surveys. In this questionnaire, for example, calligraphy, painting, photography, and Japanese dressmaking are all included within the category of creative activities. Therefore, the relationship between individual activities within that category and the maintenance of cognitive function is not clear.

Third, the MMSE is a method of evaluation that is used to screen for dementia and is not a diagnostic tool for MCI and cognitive decline. At present, various cut-off values are used for provisionally determining MCI and cognitive decline. The difference between the clinical diagnosis and the definition based on MMSE cut-off score should be considered.

Fourth, although our study was longitudinal, the possibility of causal inversion cannot be ruled out completely. Because dementia and depression are reportedly associated with each other, participation in leisure activities might be reduced in people in the early stages of cognitive impairment [58, 59]. The decrease in the frequency of a CLA might reflect the early symptoms of dementia.

Fifth, although a significant interaction was observed between LCPUFA intake and engagement in ≥ 5 CLAs, no interaction was noted between LCPUFA intake and participation in each of the four CLAs other than art appreciation. The CLAs other than art appreciation included reading newspapers, reading books, writing, and doing yard work. The participants in the present study participated in these four CLAs relatively frequently given that the aforementioned activities are familiar to older people in Japan [60,61,62]. Therefore, the significant association observed between five or more CLAs and cognitive function observed in the present study may be attributed to the addition of art appreciation to the four familiar and frequently performed intellectual activities.

Sixth, the validity of the nutritional assessment for quantifying LCPUFA intake is insufficient. Fukumoto et al. [63] reported that a seven-day dietary record is required to estimate the true value of PUFA, even within an error range of ± 15 %. We used a three-day dietary record, which may not have provided an accurate estimation of LCPUFA intake. For example, the total value of DHA and EPA was 0.963 g in our study, but Kawabata et al. [64] reported 0.90 g for men and 0.75 g for women in a 28-day dietary record survey of older adults in Japan; thus, the possibility of overestimation due to using a three-day dietary record cannot be denied. However, for ARA, the mean value for both men and women in this study was 0.161 g, and the 28-day dietary survey reported similar values of 0.15 g for women and 0.17 g for men.

Implications for practice

Similar to muscles, based on the use-it-or-lose-it theory, cognitive function can undergo a gradual decline in older people, who have increasingly fewer opportunities to use these skills. Ongoing participation in CLAs later in life might stabilize or even activate the unused aspects of cognitive functioning. A recent prospective study revealed an association between limited participation in cognitive or social activities and an increased risk of developing dementia [65]. Daily participation in CLAs may also have a positive impact on mood and emotions, in addition to preserving cognitive function. Moreover, many opportunities are available to modify the daily diet regardless of age because the diet is essential for life. It is possible that older people in Japan experience a higher intake of LCPUFAs because of the substantial role played by fish in the indigenous diet. Older people in other countries might be encouraged to include more fish in their daily diets.

Conclusions

The present study demonstrated the interaction of the high-frequency engagement in a variety of CLAs combined with high LCPUFA intake on cognitive function preservation in community-dwelling older people. In addition, one of the CLAs, i.e., art appreciation, may be associated with the preservation of cognitive function. These lifestyle factors can be modified, and CLAs in particular are better suited for older people because of their lower physical burden. Further study is required to establish a link with clinical significance, which may also provide modifiable daily interventions to maintain cognitive function.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and analyses performed as part of the current study are not publicly available due to the consent requirements of the participants. However, participants characteristics, including sex and age-stratified descriptive data, are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ARA:

Arachidonic acid

BMI:

Body mass index

CI:

Confidence intervals

CLA:

Cognitive leisure activities

DHA:

Docosahexaenoic acid

EPA:

Eicosapentaenoic acid

LCPUFA:

Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids

MCI:

Mild Cognitive Impairment

MET:

Metabolic equivalents

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

MUFA:

Monosaturated fatty acids

NILS-LSA:

National Institute for Longevity Sciences-Longitudinal Study of Aging

OR:

Odds ratio

SFA:

Saturated fatty acids

References

  1. World Health Organization. Risk reduction of cognitive decline and dementia: WHO Guidelines. https://www.who.int/mental_health/neurology/dementia/guidelines_risk_reduction/en/. Accessed 29 May 2020.

  2. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385:2255–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Andrieu S, Guyonnet S, Coley N, Cantet C, Bonnefoy M, Bordes S, et al. Effect of long-term omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation with or without multidomain intervention on cognitive function in elderly adults with memory complaints (MAPT): a randomised, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:377–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Richard E, Van den Heuvel E, Moll van Charante EP, Achthoven L, Vermeulen M, Bindels PJ, et al. Prevention of dementia by intensive vascular care (PreDIVA): a cluster-randomized trial in progress. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23:198–204.

  5. Iizuka A, Suzuki, H, Ogawa S, Kobayashi-Cuya KE, Kobayashi M, Takebayashi T, et al. Can cognitive leisure activity prevent cognitive decline in older adults? A systematic review of intervention studies. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2019;19:469–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Yates LA, Ziser S, Spector A, Orrell M. Cognitive leisure activities and future risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr. 2016;28:1791–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Xu W, Tan L, Wang HF, Jiang T, Tan MS, Tan L, et al. Meta-analysis of modifiable risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2015;86:1299–306.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Söderberg M, Edlund C, Kristensson K, Dallner G. Fatty acid composition of brain phospholipids in aging and in Alzheimer’s disease. Lipids. 1991;26:421–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. McNamara RK, Liu Y, Jandacek R, Rider T, Tso P. The aging human orbitofrontal cortex: decreasing polyunsaturated fatty acid composition and associated increases in lipogenic gene expression and stearoyl-CoA desaturase activity. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2008;78:293–304.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Sasaki H, Sueyasu T, Tokuda H, Ito M, Kaneda Y, Rogi T, et al. Aging and FADS1 polymorphisms decrease the biosynthetic capacity of long-chain PUFAs: a human trial using [U- 13 C] linoleic acid. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2019;148:1–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Fukaya T, Gondaira T, Kashiyae Y, Kotani S, Ishikura Y, Fujikawa S, et al. Arachidonic acid preserves hippocampal neuron membrane fluidity in senescent rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:1179–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Suzuki H, Park SJ, Tamura M, Ando S. Effect of the long-term feeding of dietary lipids on the learning ability, fatty acid composition of brain stem phospholipids and synaptic membrane fluidity in adult mice: a comparison of sardine oil diet with palm oil diet. Mech Ageing Dev. 1998;101:119–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kotani S, Nakazawa H, Tokimasa T, Akimoto K, Kawashima H, Toyoda-Ono Y, et al. Synaptic plasticity preserved with arachidonic acid diet in aged rats. Neurosci Res. 2003;46:453–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. McGahon B, Clements MP, Lynch MA. The ability of aged rats to sustain long-term potentiation is restored when the age-related decrease in membrane arachidonic acid concentration is reversed. Neuroscience. 1997;81:9–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. McGahon BM, Martin DS, Horrobin DF, Lynch MA. Age-related changes in synaptic function: analysis of the effect of dietary supplementation with omega-3 fatty acids. Neuroscience. 1999;94:305–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Tokuda H, Kontani M, Kawashima H, Kiso Y, Shibata H, Osumi N. Differential effect of arachidonic acid and docosahexaenoic acid on age-related decreases in hippocampal neurogenesis. Neurosci Res. 2014;88:58–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kawakita E, Hashimoto M, Shido O. Docosahexaenoic acid promotes neurogenesis in vitro and in vivo. Neuroscience. 2006;139:991–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Tokuda H, Ito M, Sueyasu T, Sasaki H, Morita S, Kaneda Y, et al. Effects of combining exercise with long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation on cognitive function in the elderly: a randomised controlled trial. Sci Rep. 2020;10:12906.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Tokuda H, Sueyasu T, Kawashima H, Shibata H, Koga Y. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation improves mood in elderly Japanese men. J Oleo Sci. 2017;66:713–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Tokuda H, Sueyasu T, Kontani M, Kawashima H, Shibata H, Koga Y. Low doses of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids affect cognitive function in elderly Japanese men: a randomized controlled trial. J Oleo Sci. 2015;64:633–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Shimokata H, Ando F, Niino N. A new comprehensive study on aging–the National Institute for Longevity Sciences, Longitudinal Study of Aging (Nils-LSA). J Epidemiol. 2000;10 Suppl 1:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Mori E, Mitani Y, Yamadori A. Usefulness of a Japanese version of the Mini Mental State Test in neurological patients (in Japanese). Jpn J Neuropsychol. 1985;1:82–90.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Nishita Y, Tange C, Tomida M, Otsuka R, Ando F, Shimokata H. Personality and global cognitive decline in Japanese community-dwelling elderly people: a 10-year longitudinal study. J Psychosom Res. 2016;91:20–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. O’Bryant SE, Humphreys JD, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Graff-Radford NR, Petersen RC, et al. Detecting dementia with the mini-mental state examination in highly educated individuals. Arch Neurol. 2008;65:963–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Nieuwenhuis-Mark RE. The death knoll for the MMSE: has it outlived its purpose? J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2010;23:151–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003;348:2508–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Kozono M, Gondo Y, Ogawa M, Ishioka YL. The relationship between leisure activities and cognitive function in community-dwelling older adults (in Japanese). Jpn J Gerontol. 2016;38:32–44.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Imai T, Sakai S, Mori K, Ando F, Niino N, Shimokata H. Nutritional assessments of 3-day dietary records in National Institute for Longevity Sciences–Longitudinal Study of Aging (NILS-LSA). J Epidemiol. 2000:10 Suppl 1:70–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Office for Resources. Policy division, Science and Technology Policy Bureau, Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology. Standard tables of foods composition in Japan 2010. https://www.mext.go.jp/b_menu/shingi/gijyutu/gijyutu3/houkoku/1298713.htm. Accessed 29 May 2020.

  31. Kozakai R, Ando F, Kim HY, Rantanen T, Shimokata H. Regular exercise history as a predictor of exercise in community-dwelling older Japanese people. J Phys Fit Sport Med. 2012;1:167–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Shima S. A new self-rating scale for depression (in Japanese). Clin Psychiatry. 1985;27:717–23.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Geda YE, Topazian HM, Roberts LA, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Pankratz VS, et al. Engaging in cognitive activities, aging, and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011;23:149–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Wang JY, Zhou DH, Li J, Zhang M, Deng J, Tang M, et al. Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: the Chongqing aging study. Neurology. 2006;66:911–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Cracchiolo JR, Mori T, Nazian SJ, Tan J, Potter H, Arendash GW. Enhanced cognitive activity–over and above social or physical activity–is required to protect Alzheimer’s mice against cognitive impairment, reduce Abeta deposition, and increase synaptic immunoreactivity. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;88:277–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA. Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging 1999;14:245–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hultsch DF, Hammer M, Small BJ. Age differences in cognitive performance in later life: relationships to self-reported health and activity life style. J Gerontol. 1993;48:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bienias JL, Evans DA, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2002;287:742–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet. 2000;355:1315–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hou XH, Xu W, Bi YL, Shen XN, Ma YH, Dong Q, et al. Associations of healthy lifestyles with cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease pathology in cognitively intact older adults: the CABLE study. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13:81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Fancourt D, Steptoe A. The art of life and death: 14 year follow-up analyses of associations between arts engagement and mortality in the English longitudinal study of ageing. BMJ 2019;367:l6377.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull. 2005;131:925–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Satoh M, Ogawa JI, Tokita T, Nakaguchi N, Nakao K, Kida H, Tomimoto H. Physical exercise with music maintains activities of daily living in patients with dementia: Mihama-Kiho project Part 21. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;57:85–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Di Dio C, Macaluso E, Rizzolatti G. The golden beauty: brain response to classical and renaissance sculptures. PloS one. 2007;2:e1201.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Kawabata H, Zeki S. Neural correlates of beauty. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:1699–705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Ishizu T, Zeki S. Toward a brain-based theory of beauty. PLoS One. 2011;6: e21852.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Wikenheiser AM, Marrero-Garcia Y, Schoenbaum G. Suppression of ventral hippocampal output impairs integrated orbitofrontal encoding of task Structure Neuron. 2017;95:1197–207.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Wang L, Liu J, Harvey-White J, Zimmer A, Kunos G. Endocannabinoid signaling via cannabinoid receptor 1 is involved in ethanol preference and its age-dependent decline in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:1393–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Berrendero F, Romero J, García-Gil L, Suarez I, De la Cruz P, Ramos JA, et al. Changes in cannabinoid receptor binding and mRNA levels in several brain regions of aged rats. Biochim Biophys Acta. 1998;1407:205–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Romero J, Berrendero F, García-Gil L, de la Cruz P, Ramos JA, Fernández-Ruiz JJ. Loss of cannabinoid receptor binding and messenger RNA levels and cannabinoid agonist-stimulated [35S]guanylyl-5’O-(thio)-triphosphate binding in the basal ganglia of aged rats. Neuroscience. 1998;4:1075–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Piyanova A, Lomazzo E, Bindila L, Lerner R, Albayram O, Ruhl T, et al. Age-related changes in the endocannabinoid system in the mouse hippocampus. Mech Ageing Dev. 2015;150:55–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Bilkei-Gorzo A, Albayram O, Draffehn A, Michel K, Piyanova A, Oppenheimer H, et al. A chronic low dose of ∆9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) restores cognitive function in old mice. Nat Med. 2017;23:782–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Korbecki J, Bobiński R, Dutka M. Self-regulation of the inflammatory response by peroxisome proliferator-activated receptors. Inflamm Res. 2019;68:443–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Cheng ST. Cognitive Reserve and the Prevention of Dementia: the Role of Physical and Cognitive Activities. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2016;18:85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Miyagawa N, Miura K, Okuda N, Kadowaki T, Takashima N, Nagasawa SY, et al. Long-chain n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids intake and cardiovascular disease mortality risk in Japanese: a 24-year follow-up of NIPPON DATA80. Atherosclerosis. 2014;232:384–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Elmadfa I, Kornsteiner M. Fats and fatty acid requirements for adults. Ann Nutr Metab. 2009;55:56–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Green RC, Cupples LA, Kurz A, Auerbach S, Go R, Sadovnick D, et al. Depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer disease: the Mirage Study. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:753–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Ownby RL, Crocco E, Acevedo A, John V, Loewenstein D. Depression and risk for Alzheimer disease: systematic review, meta-analysis, and metaregression analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:530–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Larry K. World Association of News publishers, world press trends: digital revenues won’t replace print. https://www.wan-ifra.org/press-releases/2009/12/01/world-press-trends-digital-revenues-wont-replace-print. Accessed 29 May 2020.

  61. The United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization. Reading the past, writing the future: fifty years of promoting literacy. p24. https://unesdoc.unesco.org/ark:/48223/pf0000247563. Accessed 29 May 2020.

  62. Iwasa H, Yoshida Y, Kai I, Suzuki T, Kim H, Yoshida H. Leisure activities and cognitive function in elderly community-dwelling individuals in Japan: a 5-year prospective cohort study. J Psychosom Res. 2012;72:159–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Fukumoto A, Asakura K, Murakami K, Sasaki S, Okubo H, Hirota N, et al. Within- and between-individual variation in energy and nutrient intake in Japanese adults: effect of age and sex differences on group size and number of records required for adequate dietary assessment. J Epidemiol. 2013;23:178–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Kawabata T, Hirota S, Hirayama T, Adachi N, Kaneko Y, Iwama N, et al. Associations between dietary n-6 and n-3 fatty acids and arachidonic acid compositions in plasma and erythrocytes in young and elderly Japanese volunteers. Lipids Health Dis. 2011;10:138.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Floud S, Balkwill A, Mauricio Reus E, Green J, Reeves GK, Beral V. OP50 Cognitive and social activities and long-term risk of dementia in UK women: prospective study. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2019;73 Suppl 1:24–25.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all the participants, health professionals, and researchers, as well as their colleagues in the NILS-LSA, for completing the survey for this study. We also thank those who were involved in the data collection and analyses. This work was supported by the Research Funding for Longevity Sciences from the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Japan. I would like to thank Yoshinori Kitagawa (Suntory Wellness Ltd.) for scientific advice and encouragement.

Funding

This cohort study based on the NILS-LSA was supported by the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology (899-3). The present study was completed by Suntory Wellness Ltd. CH, TT, TR, and H Shibata are employees of Suntory Wellness, Ltd.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The research was designed by CH, RO, FA, and H Shimokata; the study was implemented by HS (Hiroshi Shimokata), FA, and RO; the essential reagents or essential materials were provisioned by H Shimokata, FA, RO, YN, CT, and YK; the data and statistical analyses were carried out by CH; the manuscript was written by CH, RO, YN, CT, YK, TT, TR, H Shibata, FA, and H Shimokata; and RO was responsible for the final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rei Otsuka.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Committee of Ethics of Human Research of the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology approved this study (Approval number: 1322). All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations and informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Written informed consent to publish this paper was obtained from the participants.

Competing interests

This work was supported by Suntory Wellness Ltd., which markets health food products that include LCPUFAs. CH, TT, TR, and H Shibata are employees of Suntory Wellness Ltd. Others declare no conflict of interest regarding this study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Horikawa, C., Otsuka, R., Nishita, Y. et al. Interaction between cognitive leisure activity and long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid intake on global cognitive decline in a Japanese longitudinal cohort study: National Institute for Longevity Sciences-Longitudinal Study of Aging. BMC Geriatr 21, 443 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12877-021-02359-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12877-021-02359-8

Keywords